Copy Link
Add to Bookmark
Report

AIList Digest Volume 5 Issue 258

eZine's profile picture
Published in 
AIList Digest
 · 15 Nov 2023

AIList Digest            Tuesday, 3 Nov 1987      Volume 5 : Issue 258 

Today's Topics:
Journal Call - BBS Commentators
Bibliography - Knowledge Acquisition for Knowledge-Based Systems

----------------------------------------------------------------------

Date: 2 Nov 87 15:24:56 GMT
From: mind!harnad@princeton.edu (Stevan Harnad)
Subject: BBS Call for Commentators: 7 target articles


Below are the abstracts of seven forthcoming articles on which BBS --
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, an international, interdisciplinary Journal
of Open Peer Commentary, published by Cambridge University Press --
invites self-nominations from potential commentators. The procedure is
explained after the abstracts. The seven articles are:

(1) The Intentional Stance (Dan Dennett) [multiple book review]
(2) The Ethological Basis of Learning (A. Gardner & B. Gardner)
(3) Tactical deception in Primates (A. Whiten & R.W. Byrne)
(4) Event-Related Potentials and Memory: A Critique of the Context
Updating Hypothesis (Rolf Verleger)
(5) Is the P300 Component a Manifestation of Context Updating?
(E. Donchin & M. Coles) [article-length precommentary on (4)]
(6) Real and Depicted Spaces: A Cross-Cultural Perspective (J.B. Deregowski)
(7) Research on Self Control: An Integrating Framework (A.W. Logue)

-----

1. The Intentional Stance

Dan Dennett
Philosophy Department
Tufts university

The intentional stance is the strategy of prediction and
explanation that attributes beliefs, desires and other
"intentional" states to organisms and devices and predicts
future behavior from what it would be rational for an agent
to do, given those beliefs and desires. Any device or
organism that regularly uses this strategy is an
"intentional system," whatever its innards might be. The
strategy of treating parts of the world as intentional
systems is the foundation of "folk psychology," but it is
also exploited (and is virtually unavoidable) in artificial
intelligence and cognitive science in general, as well as in
evolutionary theory. An analysis of the role of the
intentional stance and its presuppositions supports a
naturalistic theory of mental states and events, their
"content" or "intentionality," and the relation between
"mentalistic" levels of explanation and neurophysiological
or mechanistic levels of explanation. As such, the analysis
of the intentional stance grounds a theory of the mind and
its relation to the body.

2. The Ethological Basis of Learning

A. Gardner & B. Gardner
Psychology Department
University of Nevada

One view of the basic nature of the learning process has
dominated theory and application throughout the century. It
is the view that the behavior of organisms is governed by
its positive and negative consequences. Anyone who has
attempted to use this principle to teach relatively complex
skills to free-living, well-fed subjects -- as we have done
in our sign language studies of chimpanzees -- is apt to
have been disappointed.

Meanwhile, recent ethological findings plainly contradict
the argument that most, or even much, of the learning that
takes place in the operant conditioning laboratory is based
on the "law of effect." The residue of support for the law
of effect that might be derived from operant conditioning
experiments depends entirely on the logic of a particular
experimental design. There is, however, a logical defect in
this design that cannot be repaired by any conceivable
improvement in procedure or instrumentation. However deeply
ingrained in our cultural traditions, the notion that
behavior is based on its positive consequences cannot be
supported by laboratory evidence. Several key phenomena of
conditioning can be dealt with in a more straightforward
manner by dispensing with hedonism altogether,

An impressive amount of human behavior persists, and
persists in spite of its negative consequences. The popular
notion that persistent maladaptive behavior is rare in other
animals is easily refuted by those who have observed other
animals closely in their natural habitats. We offer an
analysis of adaptive and maladaptive behavior in aversive
conditioning and of the design of experiments on the effect
of predictive contingencies in Pavlovian conditioning. The
latter attempt to demonstrate an effect of contingency fails
because it violates basic principles of experimental design.
We conclude that there is a fundamental logical defect in
all notions of contingency.

This reconsideration of the traditional behavioristic and
cognitive versions of the law of effect was originally
suggested by problems in teaching new and challenging
patterns of behavior to free-living subjects such as
children and chimpanzees, which we briefly describe in
closing.

3. Tactical Deception in Primates

A. Whiten & R.W. Byrne
Psychological Laboratories
University of St. Andrews, Scotland

Tactical deception occurs when an individual's is able to
use an "honest" act from his normal repertoire in a
different context to mislead familiar individuals. Although
primates have a reputation for social skill, most primate
groups are so intimate that any deception is likely to be
subtle and infrequent. Records are often anecdotal and not
widely known in the formal literature of behavioral science.
We have tackled this problem by drawing together records
from many primates and primatologists in order to look for
repeating patterns. This has revealed a many forms of
deceptive tactics, which we classify in terms of the
function they perform. For each class, we sketch the
features of another individual's state of mind that a
deceiver must be able to represent, acting as a "natural
psychologist." Our analysis clarifies and perhaps explains
certain taxonomic differences. Before these findings can be
generalized, however, behavioral scientists must agree on
some fundamental methodological and theoretical questions in
the study of the evolution of social cognition.

4. Event-Related Potentials and Memory:
A Critique of the Context Updating Hypothesis

Rolf Verleger
Mannheim, West Germany

P3 is the most prominent of the electrical potentials of the
human electroencephalogram that are sensitive to
psychological variables. According to the most influential
current hypothesis about its psychological significance [E.
Donchin's], the "context updating" hypothesis, P3 reflects
the updating of working memory. This hypothesis cannot
account for relevant portions of the available evidence and
it entails some basic contradictions. A more general
formulation of this hypothesis is that P3 reflects the
updating of expectancies. This version implies that P3-
evoking stimuli are initially unexpected but later become
expected. This contradiction cannot be resolved within this
formulation.

The alternative "context closure" hypothesis retains the
concept of "strategic information processing" emphasized by
the context updating hypothesis. P3s are evoked by events
that are awaited when subjects deal with repetitive, highly
structured tasks; P3s arise from subjects' combining
successive stimuli into larger units The tasks in which P3s
are elicited can accordingly be classified in terms of their
respective formal sequences of stimuli. P3 may be a
physiological indicator of excess activation being released
from perceptual control areas.

5. Is the P300 component a manifestation of Context Updating?

Emanuel Donchin and Michael G. H. Coles
Cognitive Psychophysiology Laboratory
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
[article-length precommentary on Verleger]

To understand the endogenous components of the ERP we must
use from data about the components' antecedent conditions to
form hypotheses about the information processing function of
the underlying brain activity. These hypotheses, in turn,
generate testable predictions about the consequences of the
component. We review the application of this approach to the
analysis of the P300 component, whose amplitude is
controlled multiplicatively by the subjective probability
and the task relevance of the eliciting events and whose
latency depends on the duration of stimulus evaluation.
These and other factors suggest that the P300 is a
manifestation of activity occurring whenever one's model of
the environment must be revised. Tests of three predictions
based on this "context updating" model are reviewed.
Verleger's critique is based on a misconstrual of the model
as well as on a partial and misleading reading of the
relevant literature.

6. Real and Depicted Spaces:
A Cross-Cultural Perspective

J.B. Deregowski
Psychology Department
University of Aberdeen, Scotland

This paper examines the contribution of cross-cultural
studies to our understanding of the perception and
representation of space. A cross-cultural survey of the
basic difficulties in understanding pictures -- from the
failure to recognize a picture as a representation to the
inability to recognise the object represented -- indicates
that similar difficulties occur in pictorial and
nonpictorial cultures. Real and pictorial spaces must be
distinguished. The experimental work on pictorial space
derives from two distinct traditions: the study of picture
perception in "remote" populations and the study of
perceptual illusions. A comparison of the findings on
pictorial space perception with those on real space
perception and perceptual constancies suggests that cross-
cultural differences in the perception of both real and
depicted space involve two different kinds of skills: those
related only to real spaces or only to depicted spaces and
those related to both. Different cultural groups use
different skills to perform the same perceptual task.

7. Research on Self Control: An Integrating Framework

A.W. Logue
Department of Psychology
SUNY - Stony Brook

The tendency to choose a larger, more delayed reinforcer
over a smaller, less delayed one (self-control) depends on
the current physical values of the reinforcers. It also
varies according to a subject's experience and current
factors other than the reinforcers. Two local delay models
(Mischel's social learning theory and Herrnstein's matching
law) as well as molar maximization models have taken into
account these indirect effects on self control by
representing a subject's behavior as a function of a
perceived environment. A general evolutionary analysis of
all this research yields a better and more predictive
description of self control.


-----


This is an experiment in using the Net to find eligible commentators for
articles in Behavioral & Brain Sciences. [...]
Eligible individuals who judge that they would have a relevant
commentary to contribute should contact me at the e-mail address indicated at
the bottom of this message, or should write by normal mail to:

Stevan Harnad, Editor, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20 Nassau Street, Room 240
Princeton NJ 08542 (phone: 609-921-7771)

"Eligibility" usually means being an academically trained professional
contributor to one of the disciplines mentioned earlier, or to related academic
disciplines. The letter should indicate the candidate's general qualifications
as well as their basis for wishing to serve as commentator for the particular
target article in question. It is preferable also to enclose a Curriculum Vitae.
(Please note that the editorial office must exercise selectivity among the
nominations received so as to ensure a strong and balanced cross-specialty
spectrum of eligible commentators.) [...]

Stevan Harnad harnad@mind.princeton.edu (609)-921-7771

------------------------------

Date: 29 Oct 87 17:31:44 GMT
From: mcvax!ukc!reading!onion!spb@uunet.uu.net (Stephen)
Subject: Bibliography - Knowledge Acquisition for Knowledge-Based Systems


Proceedings of the first
European Workshop on

KNOWLEDGE ACQUISITION FOR
KNOWLEDGE - BASED SYSTEMS


Co - Sponsored by the
Institution of Electrical Engineers
2nd - 3rd September 1987
Reading University


There are only a limited number of Proceedings. These are
available on a first come first served basis. The cost will
be 35 pounds sterling, which includes post and packing
within the UK. Cheques should be made payable to 'The
University of Reading'.

Orders to: Professor T R Addis
Department of Computer Science
University of Reading
Whiteknights
Reading
RG6 2AX


BIBLIOGRAPHY



Broy, M., "Transformational Semantics for Concurrent Programs,"
Information Processing Letters, vol. 11, pp. 87-91, 1980.

Evans, D.J. and Shirley A Williams, "Analysis and Detection of
Parallel Processable Code," Computer Journal, vol. 23, pp.
66-72, 1980.

Kuck, D.J., in The Structure of Computers and Computations, vol.
1, John Wiley and Sons, 1978.

Roucairol, G., "Transformations of Sequential Programs into
Parallel Programs," Cambridge University Press, 1982.

Foster, C C, "Information storage and retrieval using AVL trees,"
ACM 20th National conference, 1965.

Knowlton, K C, "A fast storage allocator," CACM, vol. 8, no. 10,
pp. 623-625, October 1965.

Deuel, P, "On a storage mapping function for data structures,"
CACM, vol. 9, no. 5, May 1966.

Knowlton, K C, "A programmer's description of llllll," CACM, vol.
9, no. 8, Aug. 1966.

CODASYL, ACM, NY, April, 1971.

On Conceptual Modelling. Perspectives from Artificial Intelli-
gence, Databases and Programming Languages, Topics in Infor-
mation Systems, Springer-Verlag, 1984.

"Prolog-2 Reference Manual," 9 West Way, Oxford, OC2 0JB, UK, Ex-
pert Systems International Ltd., 1985.

Quintus Prolog Reference Manual, 6, Quintus Computer Systems
Inc., 1986.

"Arity/Prolog: The Programming Language," 358 Baker Avenue, Con-
cord MA 01742, USA, Arity Corporation, 1986.

Addis, T.R., "A Relation-Based Language Interpreter for a Content
Addressable File Store," ACM Trans on Database Systems, vol.
7, no. 2, pp. 125-163, 1982.

Addis, T.R., "Knowledge Refining for a Diagnostic Aid," Interna-
tional Journal of Man-Machine Studies, vol. 17, pp. 151-164,
1982.

Addis, T.R., Designing Knowledge-Based Systems, Kogan Page, 1985.
ISBN0-85038-859-7

Addis, T.R., "The Role of Explanation in Knowledge Elicitation,"
International Journal of Systems Research and Information
Science, vol. 2, pp. 101-110, 1986.

Addis, T.R., The Boundaries of Knowledge, Informatics 9, 1987.
ASLIB Conference at Kings College, Cambridge

Rawlings, C.J., Representing protein structures in Prolog: the
Prolog representation, Imperial Cancer Research Fund,
Biomedical Computing Unit, 1986. Submitted as part of
results of SERC Contract No: SO/351/84

Hamm, G.H. and G.N. Cameron, "The EMBL data library," Nucleic
Acids Research, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 5-10, 1986.

Chothia, C., "Principles that determine the structure of pro-
teins," Annual Reviews of Biochemistry, vol. 53.

Codd, E.F., "A relational model of data for large shared data
banks," Comm. ACM, pp. 377-387, 1970.

Codd, E.F., "Further normalization of the database relational
model," IBM Research report, 1971. IBM Thomas Watson
Research Centre. N.Y.

Bridge, D., "Conceptual Data Models in Database Design," Final
year project report for BSc Computer Science at Brunel
University, 1986.

Kyte, J. and R.F. Doolittle, "A simple method for displaying the
hydropathic character of a protein," Journal of Molecular
Biology, vol. 157, pp. 105-132, 1982.

Duncan, T., PROPS 2 Reference Manual, Imperial Cancer Research
Fund, Biomedical Computing Unit, 1986.

Sweet, R.M. and D. Eisenberg, "Correlation of sequence hydropho-
bicities measures similarity in three dimensional protein
structure," Journal of Molecular Biology, vol. 171, pp.
479-488, 1983.

Elleby, P. and T.R. Addis, "Extending the Relational Database
Model to capture more Constraints," A KSG Technical Report,
1987.

Chou, P.Y. and G.D. Fasman, "Prediction of the secondary struc-
ture of proteins from their amino acid sequence," Advances
in Enzymology, vol. 47, pp. 45-148, 1980.

Ptitsyn, O.B. and A.V. Finkelstein, "Similarities of protein to-
pologies: evolutionary divergence - functional convergence
or principles of folding?," Annual Reviews of Biophysics,
vol. 13, pp. 339-386, 1980.

Bernstein, F.C., T. Koetzle, G.J.B. William, E. Meyer, M.D.
Brice, J.R. Rodger, O. Kennard, T. Shimanouchi, and M.
Tasumi, "The protein data bank: a computer-based archival
file for macromolecular structures," Journal of Molecular
Biology, vol. 112, pp. 535-542, 1977.

Harre, R., The Philosophy of Science: An Introductory Survey, Ox-
ford University Press, 1976.

George, D.G., W.C. Barker, and L.T. Hunt, "The protein informa-
tion resource (PIR)," Nucleic Acids Research, vol. 14, no.
1, pp. 17-20, 1986.

Cohen, F.E., R.M. Abarbanel, I.D. Kuntz, and R.J. Fletterick,
"Secondary structure assignment for A/B proteins by a com-
binatorial approach," Biochemistry, vol. 22, pp. 4894-4904,
1983.

Rawlings, C.J., W.R. Taylor, J. Nyakairu, J. Fox, and M.J.E.
Sternberg, "Reasoning about protein topology using the logic
programming language PROLOG," Journal of Molecular Graphics,
vol. 3, pp. 151-157, 1985.

Rawlings, C.J., W.R.T. Taylor, J. Nyakairu, J. Fox, and M.J.E.
Sternberg, Using Prolog to Represent and Reason about Pro-
tein Structure, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, p. 536,
Springer-Verlag, 1986.

Bruner, J.S., J.J. Goodnow, and G.A. Austin, in A Study of Think-
ing, Wiley, 1956.

Maizel, J. and R.P. Lenk, "Enhanced graphic matrix analysis of
nucleic acid and protein sequences," Proceedings of the Na-
tional Academy of Science USA, vol. 78, no. 12, pp. 7665-
7669, 1981.

Lim, V.I., "Structural principles of the globular organization of
protein chains. A sterochemical theory of globular protein
secondary structure," Journal of Molecular Biology, vol. 88,
pp. 857-872, 1974.

Bolton, N., in Concept Formation, Pergamon Press, 1977. ISBN 0-
08-0214940

Chen, P.P., "The Entity Relationship Model: Toward a Unified View
of Data," ACM Trans on Data Base Systems, vol. 1, no. 1, pp.
9-13, 1976.

Peirce, C.S., Charles S. Peirce: Selected Writings, Dover Publi-
cations Inc, 1966.

Kowalski, R., Logic for Problem Solving, Artificial Intelligence
Series, North Holland Press, Amsterdam, 1979.

Richardson, J., "B-sheet topology and the relatedness of pro-
teins," Nature, vol. 268, pp. 495-500, 1977.

Richardson, J., "The anatomy and taxonomy of protein structure,"
Advances in Protein Chemistry, vol. 34, pp. 167-339, 1981.

Garnier, J., D.J. Osguthorpe, and B. Robson, "Analysis of the ac-
curacy and implications of simple methods for predicting the
secondary structure of globular proteins," Journal of Molec-
ular Biology, vol. 120, pp. 97-120, 1978.

Kabsch, W. and C.Sander, "How good are predictions of protein
secondary structure?," FEBS Letters, vol. 155, pp. 179-182,
1983.

Blundell, T. and M.J.E. Sternberg, "Computer-aided design in pro-
tein engineering," Trends in biotechnology, vol. 3, pp.
228-235, 1985.

Fox, J., D. Frost, T. Duncan, and N. Preston, The PROPS 2 Primer,
Imperial Cancer Research Fund, Biomedical Computing Unit,
1986.

Eisenberg, D., R.M. Weiss, T.C. Terwilliger, and W. Wilcox, "Hy-
drophobic moments and protein structure," Faraday Symposia
Chemical Society, vol. 17, pp. 109-120, 1982.

Taylor, W.R., Protein Structure Prediction, A Practical Approach,
IRL, Oxford, 1987.

Cohen, F.E., M.J.E. Sternberg, and W.R. Taylor, "Analysis and
prediction of protein B-sheet structures by a combinatorial
approach," Nature, vol. 285, pp. 378-382, 1980.

Cohen, F.E., M.J.E. Sternberg, and W.R. Taylor, "Analysis and
prediction of the packing of B-sheet in the tertiary struc-
ture of globular proteins," Journal of Molecular Biology,
vol. 156, pp. 821-862, 1982.

Sternberg, M.J.E. and J.M. Thornton, "On the conformation of pro-
teins: the handiness of the connection between parallel B-
strands," Journal of Molecular Biology, vol. 110, pp. 269-
283, 1977.

Taylor, W.R. and J.M. Thornton, "Prediction of super-secondary
structure in proteins," Nature, vol. 301, pp. 540-542, 1983.

Burridge, J.M., A.J. Morffew, and S.J.P. Todd, "Experiments in
the use of PROLOG for protein querying," Journal of Molecu-
lar Graphics, vol. 3, p. 109, 1985. abstract 13

Lim, V.I., "Algorithms for prediction of A-helical and B-
structural regions in globular proteins," Journal of Molecu-
lar Biology, vol. 88, pp. 873-894, 1974.

Bobrow, D. and T. Wingrad, "An Overview of KRL, a Knowledge
Representation Language," Cognitive Science, vol. 1, no. 1,
1977.

Hopp, T.P. and K.R. Woods, "A computer program for predicting an-
tigenic determinants," Molecular Immunology, vol. 20, 1983.

Grant, T.J. and P. Elleby, An AI Aid for Scheduling Repair Jobs,
pp. 20-22, Paris, 1986. Conference of the Association Fran-
caise d'Intelligence et des Systems de Simulation

Sowa, J.F., Conceptual Structures: Information processing in mind
and machine, Addison-Wesley, 1984.

V.Begg,, Developing Expert CAD Systems, Kogan Page, 1984.

Ullman, J.D., Principles of Database Systems, Pitman Publishing,
1985.

Brueker, J.A. and B.J. Wielings, "Analysis Techniques for
Knowledge Based Systems," Part 2 Esprit Project 12 1.2,
University of Amsterdam, 1983.

Fikes, R. and T. Kehler, "The Role of Frame-Based Representation
in Reasoning," September Communication of the ACM, vol. 28,
no. 9, pp. 904-920, 1985.

Date, C J, An Introduction to Database Systems, Addison-Wesley,
1981.

Hendrix, G G, "Partitioned Networks for Mathematical Modelling of
Natural Language Semantics," Technical Report NL-28, 1975.
Department of Computer Science, University of Texas

Lakatos, I, "The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes,"
Philosophical Papers, vol. 1, Cambridge University Press,
1978.

Lee, B, "Introducing Systems Analysis and Design," NCC, vol. I &
II, Manchester, 1978.

Pask, G, Conversation Theory: Applications in Education and Ep-
istemology, Oxford, 1976.

Phillips, B, A model for Knowledge and its Application to
Discourse Analysis, 1978. University of Illinois, Depart-
ment of Information Engineering KSL-9

Popper, K R, The Logic of Scientific Discovery, 1959. Hutchinson
10th impression 1980

Robinson, H, Database Analysis and Design, Chartwell-Bratt, 1981.

Rock-Evans, R, "Data Analysis," IPC Business Press, 1981.

Welbank, M, A review of Knowledge Acquisition Techniques for Ex-
pert Systems, 1983. British Telecommunications Martlesham
Consultancy Services

Wood-Harper, A T and C Fitzgerald, "A taxonomy of current ap-
proaches to systems analysis," Computer Journal, vol. 24,
no. 1, 1982.


--
******************************************************************************
* Stephen Bull * Phone: (0734) 875123 *
* Dept. of Computer Science * mail: bull@onion.reading.ac.uk *
* University of Reading, ENGLAND * *

------------------------------

End of AIList Digest
********************

← previous
next →
loading
sending ...
New to Neperos ? Sign Up for free
download Neperos App from Google Play
install Neperos as PWA

Let's discover also

Recent Articles

Recent Comments

Neperos cookies
This website uses cookies to store your preferences and improve the service. Cookies authorization will allow me and / or my partners to process personal data such as browsing behaviour.

By pressing OK you agree to the Terms of Service and acknowledge the Privacy Policy

By pressing REJECT you will be able to continue to use Neperos (like read articles or write comments) but some important cookies will not be set. This may affect certain features and functions of the platform.
OK
REJECT